Research Article Journal of Orthoptera Research 2021, 30(1): 43-50 Substrate-borne vibration in Pacific field cricket courtship displays E Date Broper', AARON W. Wikte!*", JAMES H. GALLAGHER4, ROBIN M. TINGHITELLA? 1 St. Ambrose University, Biology, Davenport, IA 52803, USA. 2 University of Denver, Biology, Denver, CO 80110, USA. Corresponding author: E Dale Broder (edalebroder@gmail.com) Academic editor: Diptarup Nandi | Received 30 October 2019 | Accepted 10 August 2020 | Published 7 May 2021 http://zoobank. org/AD93850E-DDC7-4F6B-AF2C-1053F7486617 Citation: Broder ED, Wikle AW, Gallagher JH, Tinghitella RM (2021) Substrate-borne vibration in Pacific field cricket courtship displays. Journal of Orthoptera Research 30(1): 43-50. https://doi.org/10.3897/jor.30.47778 Abstract While thought to be widely used for animal communication, sub- strate-borne vibration is relatively unexplored compared to other modes of communication. Substrate-borne vibrations are important for mating decisions in many orthopteran species, yet substrate-borne vibration has not been documented in the Pacific field cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus. Male T. oceanicus use wing stridulation to produce airborne calling songs to at- tract females and courtship songs to entice females to mate. A new male morph has been discovered, purring crickets, which produce much quieter airborne calling and courtship songs than typical males. Purring males are largely protected from a deadly acoustically orienting parasitoid fly, and they are still able to attract female crickets for mating though typical call- ing song is more effective for attracting mates. Here, we document the first record of substrate-borne vibration in both typical and purring male morphs of T. oceanicus. We used a paired microphone and accelerometer to simultaneously record airborne and substrate-borne sounds produced during one-on-one courtship trials in the field. Both typical and purring males produced substrate-borne vibrations during courtship that tempo- rally matched the airborne acoustic signal, suggesting that the same mech- anism (wing movement) produces both sounds. As previously established, in the airborne channel, purring males produce lower amplitude but higher peak frequency songs than typical males. In the vibrational chan- nel, purring crickets produce songs that are higher in peak frequency than typical males, but there is no difference in amplitude between morphs. Because louder songs (airborne) are preferred by females in this species, the lack of difference in amplitude between morphs in the substrate-borne channel could have implications for mating decisions. This work lays the groundwork for investigating variation in substrate-borne vibrations in T. oceanicus, intended and unintended receiver responses to these vibrations, and the evolution of substrate-borne vibrations over time in conjunction with rapid evolutionary shifts in the airborne acoustic signal. Keywords communication, purring crickets, Teleogryllus oceanicus Introduction Natural and sexual selection have created complex and beauti- ful signals through which organisms communicate. These signals * both authors contributed equally are presented in a broad spectrum of sensory modalities, ranging from visual, such as the colorful dances of male jumping spiders, to chemical, like the sweet scent flowers produce to attract pollina- tors. One of the oldest, yet least understood, modes of communi- cation is substrate-borne vibration, in which vibrations are sent and carried through a substrate (e.g., the stem ofa leaf or dirt) toa receiver (Hill 2009, Cocroft et al. 2014). The ubiquitous presence of vibrosensory systems, including campaniform sensilla, hair sensilla, and chordotonal organs, in nearly all insects supports the ancient origin of mechanosensory communication (Hoy and Robert 1996, Lakes-Harlan and StrouB 2014). Vibrational commu- nication is part of the auditory mode of communication, but it is a discrete channel because of the difference in media through which, and types of waves by which, vibrations are propagated. Auditory signals are primarily sent through a fluid medium like air as longitudinal pressure waves and typically travel long dis- tances, while vibrational signals are primarily sent through a solid medium, are often characterized by low-frequencies, and gener- ally travel relatively short distances as boundary waves, specifically Rayleigh waves through the ground or boundary waves through plants (Cokl and Virant-Doberlet 2003, Caldwell 2014, Hill et al. 2019). Unlike sounds that travel through air, body size does not constrain pitch for vibrational signals (except for those produced via tremulation), meaning that a small animal can potentially produce a very lowfrequency substrate-borne vibration (Cocroft and Rodriguez 2005, Caldwell 2014). Due to the human perceptual bias for airborne sound and technological limitations, we learned of the ubiquity of substrate- borne vibrations in animal communication relatively recently (Vi- rant-Doberlet et al. 2019). It is estimated that upwards of 200,000 species of insects use substrate-borne vibrations in both inter- and intra-specific communication (Cocroft and Rodriguez 2005, Hill et al. 2019). Mechanisms by which such communicative vibrations are produced are diverse and include, but are not limited to, per- cussion, stridulation, and tremulation (Cokl and Virant-Doberlet 2003, Hill 2009). For example, tremulation—moving the whole body without touching the substrate—is common in orthopterans (e.g., in the bush cricket Onomarchus uninotatus (Serville, 1838), JOURNAL OF ORTHOPTERA RESEARCH 2021, 30(1) 44 Rajaraman et al. 2015, 2018, and in katydids, Morris et al. 1994; reviewed in Stritih and Cokl 2014) and can be important for mate choice (e.g., in groundhopper Tetix ceperoi (Bolivar, 1887), Kocarek 2010; in katydid Conocephalus nigropleurum (Bruner, 1891), de Luca and Morris 1998). Adding further complexity, while many insect species rely exclusively on substrate-borne vibrations for commu- nication, many others employ a combination of both substrate- borne vibrations and other signaling modalities, either with each individual component presenting unique information or with both components providing information relative to the other (He- bets and Papaj 2005, Higham and Hebets 2013, Caldwell 2014). Airborne sound is a likely modality to be paired with substrate- borne vibrations, as the production of airborne sound unavoid- ably excites energy in the substrate-borne vibrational channel, whether via direct coupling of the signaler’s body to the substrate or induction of airborne waves to the substrate (e.g., Stdlting et all. 2002, Caldwell 2014). Stridulation, in which two body parts are rubbed together to produce sound, is the primary mechanism of both vibratory and airborne signal production in numerous insect and arachnid spe- cies and often functions in intersexual (e.g., Elias et al. 2010), intrasexual (e.g., Hill and Shadley 1997), and interspecific com- munication such as aposematic warnings (e.g., Masters 1979). Among orthopteran species, stridulation is typically associated with the production of an acoustic signal; however, in several spe- cies, stridulation has also been shown to simultaneously produce substrate-borne vibrations that function in signaler localization (in the field cricket Gryllus bimaculatus De Geer, 1773, Weide- mann and Keuper 1987 and the bush cricket Tettigonia cantans (Fuessly, 1775), Latimer and Schatral 1983) and territory estab- lishment among males (in bush crickets Tettigonia viridissima (Linnaeus, 1758), Schatral et al. 1985 and T. cantans, Keuper and Kithne 1983). Despite the increasing effort to document substrate-borne vibration in Orthoptera, we have just begun, and most species remain unexplored (Cocroft and Rodriguez 2005, Benediktov 2009). Pacific field crickets, Teleogryllus oceanicus (Le Guillou, 1841), signal in multiple modalities, including using stridulation to produce an airborne signal, but substrate-borne vibration has not been documented. Male T: oceanicus use an airborne acoustic calling song to attract females from a distance and then produce a different airborne acoustic courtship song in close one-on-one encounters with females. Females use these courtship songs and chemical signals from cuticular hydrocarbons to make mate choice decisions (Balakrishnan and Pollack 1997, Pascoal et al. 2017). In the Hawaiian Islands, the typical airborne acous- tic calling song also attracts an acoustically orienting predator, the parasitoid fly Ormia ochracea (Bigot, 1889). O. ochracea fa- cilitated the evolution of an obligately silent male morph of T. oceanicus (Zuk and Kolluru 1998, Zuk et al. 2006) and is like- ly playing a role in the evolution of a newly discovered male morph, purring crickets (Tinghitella et al. 2018, Tinghitella et al. 2021). Purring crickets are a new acoustic morph in Hawaii in which males use wing stridulation to produce airborne acoustic signals that are lower in amplitude and more broadband than typical male songs. The mean peak frequency is higher for purr- ing males than typical males, and there is more variation in peak frequency among purring males compared to typical males (Tin- ghitella et al. 2018). Phonotaxis experiments have revealed that female crickets use the purring song to locate mates, and the role of the purr in courtship is still uncertain (Tinghitella et al. 2018). Some female O. ochracea can also locate hosts using the purr, E.D. BRODER, A.W. WIKLE, J.H. GALLAGHER AND R.M. TINGHITELLA but they overwhelmingly prefer typical males in field choice tests (Tinghitella et al. 2021). The first objective of this study was to investigate the presence of substrate-borne vibrations in purring and typical T. oceanicus courtship songs. We hypothesized that T: oceanicus males generate substrate-borne vibrations during courtship as a result of the energy generated via stridulation propagating through both the air and substrate. Caldwell (2014) stated that any acoustic signaler that is in contact with a substrate will also produce a substrate-borne signal as a byproduct of the airborne signal. Since purring crickets were just discovered, this investigation is timely because shifts to substrate- borne communication channels have been previously associated with a reduction in airborne signals. In a famous example, it was hypothesized that Panamanian katydids evolved an attenuated song in response to acoustically orienting predators, and that this change in song was coupled with an increase in vibrational signals (Bel- wood and Morris 1987, Morris et al. 1994). There are also examples of animals shifting signals into vibrational channels in response to abiotic factors like darkness (Partan 2017). In our study system, the purring morph has reduced amplitude in the airborne channel (Tin- ghitella et al. 2018), likely due, in part, to selective pressure from an acoustically orienting parasitoid (Zuk and Kolluru 1998). Our second objective was to compare the amplitude and peak frequency of substrate-borne vibrations between purring and typical males. Because airborne acoustic signals (both calling song and court- ship song) differ in peak frequency and amplitude between typical and purring male morphs (Tinghitella et al. 2018), we hypothesized that substrate-borne vibrations produced via stridulation would also differ in frequency and amplitude between male morphs. Specifi- cally, typical courtship song has a lower peak frequency (median = 5.0 kHz) than purring courtship song (median = 7.6 kHz) in the air- borne channel (Tinghitella et al. 2018). Finally, we hypothesized that the mechanism of substrate-borne sound production was through wing movement during stridulation, as seen in other orthopterans (Keuper and Kiihne 1983, Weidemann and Keuper 1987). If sub- strate-borne vibrations and airborne sounds are produced through the same mechanism (wing movement during stridulation), then the two sounds should have matching temporal patterns. Material and methods We traveled to Hawaii to record substrate-borne vibration pro- duced by wild-caught male Teleogryllus oceanicus during courtship. After discovering purring crickets on Moloka‘i in 2016 and noting the presence of substrate-borne vibration at that time, we began measuring vibrations in the field in 2017. We refined our methods and began recording both vibrational and airborne acoustic songs simultaneously during field seasons in June 2018, December 2018, and June 2019. We conducted this study alongside a larger survey of courtship behavior across four islands (Hawaii, O’ahu, Moloka‘i, and Kauai) that included many populations of both typical and purring male morphs (unpublished). For a subset of these courtship trials, we used a simultaneous recording technique to record both air-borne and substrate-borne songs from 13 typi- cal males and 14 purring males. The collection of animals and courtship trials were conducted identically on all islands and on all occasions. We collected adult males and females from grassy disturbed areas (lawns) and housed them, separated by sex, in 27 x 39 x 17 cm plastic containers. We provided rabbit food, egg cartons for shelter, and moist cotton for water. After at least 48 hours of isolation from the opposite sex, we randomly selected one male and one female for each courtship JOURNAL OF ORTHOPTERA RESEARCH 2021, 30(1) E.D. BRODER, A.W. WIKLE, J.H. GALLAGHER AND R.M. TINGHITELLA Typical male \ | no He ee i cecnutaritttleake ih o =p 16500 Dati ian sly agen init Rao ct \ | , ~ I : nity 4 t = Oo VC TTL LAE La Vie 3 | ee oO & 11000 O 3 Sa TTT TOTTI LL) a ea Ld aaa | LL Ta — £ bn | ota ele Aoki ieee 5500 | ital 11 hype A, 600 ® Cc Soe Og O =z 0 I o 8 | J ee | } oy a) ® 200 | i! YOu | O We 4 2 i Will | D MM a adi, BD 1 2 3 Time (s) Frequency (Hz) 45 Purring male | | I I ‘ Mt 4 ij | H i) Nt Frequency (Hz) | .~ o o N i=] o 1 Lt) i i | ul ! Time (s) Fig. 1. Representative spectrograms showing the same song in an airborne channel (A and B) and a substrate-borne vibrational channel (C and D) fora typical male (A and C) and a purring male (B and D). Time is shown on the x-axis (seconds), and the colors (purple < red < orange < yellow) represent the power present at the various frequencies shown on the y-axis. trial. The male and female paired in each courtship trial were always from the same population and were assayed on the island from which they were collected. We measured the width of the pronotum of each individual using digital calipers, and then placed both ani- mals in a 1.5-L deli cup equipped with recording gear. Since some purring males produce very low-amplitude songs (Tinghitella et al. 2018), we began recording at the first visual observation of stridu- lation. The wing posture and motor behavior during stridulation is the same in the derived silent morph as it is in ancestral-typi- cal males (Schneider et al. 2018) as well as in the purring morph (unpublished). Recordings lasted 10 minutes after the male began stridulating. If mounting occurred, we disrupted the copulation and returned animals to group housing separated by sex so as not to influence mating in the wild population. Females and males were never used more than once in courtship trials. After completing all trials, we released animals back into their natal grassy fields. In order to record the substrate-borne vibrational and airborne components of the courtship song, we designed a courtship experi- mental container (deli cup) that used an accelerometer and a micro- phone to record both components simultaneously as separate au- dio tracks. This allowed us to determine whether the auditory signal and vibrational component were coupled and produced through the same mechanism of wing movement. Because these recordings took place in the field across islands, an accelerometer was the most portable and effective option for recording substrate-borne vibra- tions. To record vibrations, we attached an accelerometer (Knowles Acoustics, BU series 1771-000) to a piece of circular filter paper that fit perfectly in the bottom of the round 1.5-L deli cup (following Di- erkes and Barth 1995). The cord from the accelerometer fit through a small hole in the wall of the cup and traveled through a custom converter before entering a dual-input Roland Rubix 22 audio in- terface. The Roland Rubix audio interface box allows two inputs (in this case, the substrate-borne input and a simultaneously recorded audio track) and was attached to a laptop computer. For the airborne signal, we used a Rode NTG2 Multi-Powered Condenser Shotgun Microphone mounted 10 cm above the filter paper. The gain was set to 80% for both the microphone and accelerometer inputs. We recorded both tracks simultaneously. For a subset of observations, we also video-recorded trials to verify that the visual stridulation matched the audio and vibrational tracks we recorded. Because all trials took place with only red light in a dark room, we used a low- light action video camera (SiOnyx Aurora IR night vision camera). After collecting recordings in the field, we uploaded WAV files into Audacity for analysis (version 2.3.0, https://www.au- dacityteam.org). To capture variation within each male’s court- ship song, we located and analyzed three songs within each male’s recording: the first and second complete songs within the first continuous bout of calling and the last complete song within the final continuous bout of calling. For all song analyses, we used the same three songs from each male. In order to test our hypothesis that males produce substrate- borne vibrations using the same mechanism—wing movement— that they use to produce airborne acoustic signals, we first meas- ured the temporal components of both tracks. In Audacity, we visually identified the chirp and trill portions of the song; these two distinct sections of courtship song were visible in both the acoustic track and the vibrational track in both male typical and purring morphs (Fig. 1). Following Simmons et al. (2010), we JOURNAL OF ORTHOPTERA RESEARCH 2021, 30(1) 46 measured several features of each song in milliseconds: the total chirp length, the interval of silence between the chirp and the trill, the total trill length, and the trill/chirp interval. We also noted the start time within the recording for each song (three per male) for both the microphone track and the accelerometer track to verify that they matched. We used Audacity to measure the peak frequency and am- plitude of both the purring and typical songs. We analyzed the microphone tracks separately from the accelerometer tracks. One challenge in our data set was the fact that both males and females move nearly continuously for the duration of courtship interac- tions, producing broadband noise that overlapped the low-end frequencies visible in the accelerometer track. To ensure we did not disrupt normal male and female courtship behavior, we chose not to tether animals or have males court dead females; instead, we removed sections of the audio recordings that contained noise associated with locomotion after confirming that we could unam- biguously identify these parts of the recordings using the video- recorded courtship trials. For audio tracks, we analyzed the entire trill portion of the three songs for each male after removing broad- band noise associated with locomotion and applying a high-pass filter that removed all frequencies below 1500 Hz. For the acceler- ometer track, we used the same three songs for each male and se- lected the longest section of the trill portion of each song that was not interrupted by locomotor noise. For the accelerometer track, we applied a low-pass filter that removed all frequencies above 1000 Hz. We then used the plot spectrum function (settings: Han- ning window, size = 2,048, log frequency axis) to extract peak fre- quency and the contrast function to extract the amplitude (values acquired as root mean squared (RMS) in dB) of each song relative to ambient noise. We used separately recorded background noise A Microphone track eq 5 S Typical Purring C Accelerometer track Substrate-borne peak frequency (Hz) o oO Typical Purring Peak frequency (Hz) E.D. BRODER, A.W. WIKLE, J.H. GALLAGHER AND R.M. TINGHITELLA in each recording space as a baseline of 0 dB. Decibels run on a logarithmic scale, so we converted dB to a linear scale (amplitude ratio) to accurately compare amplitude among songs. This am- plitude measure is called linear amplitude, and it does not have a unit of measure (hereafter referred to as amplitude). To analyze these data, we modeled the channels (airborne microphone track and substrate-borne accelerometer track) sepa- rately with morph (purring or typical) as the main effect in each model. Because we analyzed three songs per male, we included individual ID as a random effect nested within morph (typical or purring) in each two-way ANOVA. We ran repeated measures two- way ANOVAs for the four dependent variables: airborne peak fre- quency, airborne amplitude, substrate-borne peak frequency, and substrate-borne amplitude. We used the mean and standard devia- tion of the airborne and substrate-borne frequency and amplitude data sets to calculate effect sizes using Cohen’s D (Cohen 1977). Next, to compare temporal patterns between channels (airborne and substrate-borne), we compared the two channels for a given male and a given song using paired t-tests for the three temporal measures: trill length, chirp length, and the interval between the chirp and trill. All analyses were conducted in JMP (JMP Version 14). Results As hypothesized, we detected substrate-borne vibrations in the courtship songs of male T: oceanicus (Fig. 1). We recorded sub- strate-borne vibrations in every male that we measured, including both typically singing males and purring males. When comparing purring and typical males, the airborne acoustic signals differed in the ways previously demonstrated. As 140 B Airborne amplitude oO) Oo 0 cee es L Typical Purring 12 D o_h oO -——H co a i) Substrate-borne amplitude [e>) oO Typical Purring Linear amplitude Fig. 2. Bar graphs showing the mean (equal to least squares means) and standard error (error bars) for different measures of song in typical (light gray) and purring (dark gray) male T: oceanicus. A. Peak frequency of airborne acoustic signals recorded with a micro- phone; B. Linear amplitude of airborne acoustic signals recorded with a microphone. C. Peak frequency of substrate-borne vibrations recorded with an accelerometer; D. Linear amplitude of substrate-borne vibrations recorded with an accelerometer. The asterisk indi- cates p<0.001. Effect sizes (Cohen's D) are as follows: A. 1.83, B. 4.46, C. 1.05, and D. 0.27. JOURNAL OF ORTHOPTERA RESEARCH 2021, 30(1) E.D. BRODER, A.W. WIKLE, J.H. GALLAGHER AND R.M. TINGHITELLA in Tinghitella et al. (2018), the average frequency was higher for purring males (mean + SE: 5955.8 + 178.7 Hz) than typical males (4723.3 + 185.4 Hz, F,.= 22.9, p < 0.0001; Fig. 2A), and the aver- age amplitude was lower for purring males (3.1 + 6.0) than typical males (115.9 + 6.2, F,, = 172.32, p < 0.0001; Fig. 2B). When we compared substrate-borne vibrations, we found a similar pattern for frequency, but not for amplitude. Substrate-borne vibrations in purring males were higher in peak frequency (118.0 + 4.8 Hz) than typical males (85.5 + 5.0 Hz, F,,= 22.40, p < 0.0001; Fig. 2C). There was no difference in amplitude between morphs in the sub- strate-borne channel (purring = 8.9 + 1.0, typical = 10.1 + 1.1; F,, = 0.601, p = 0.45; Fig. 2D). The size of the effects between purr- ing and typical males were as follows: airborne peak frequency = 1.83, airborne amplitude = 4.46, substrate-borne peak frequency = 1.05, and substrate-borne amplitude = 0.27. Finally, we predicted that wing movement produced both the airborne signals and substrate-borne vibrations. For all songs analyzed, the start time in milliseconds was a perfect match for the microphone and accelerometer recordings. The temporal pat- tern also matched when we compared simultaneously recorded airborne signals and substrate-borne vibrations (for example, see Fig. 1). Of the 81 songs analyzed, only two individuals were not an exact match for chirp length (0.001 and 0.002 milliseconds off). Similarly, only six individuals were not an exact match when we compared trill length and seven when we compared the interval between the trill and the chirp. In each case, the difference was less than 0.002 milliseconds. In paired t-tests, there was no differ- ence between the airborne and substrate-borne vibrations in chirp length (t,, = 0.44, p = 0.66), trill length (t,, = -0.80, p = 0.42), and the interval between the trill and the chirp (t,, = 0.76, p = 0.45). Oz = 3 O § Oo 3 a | Qe @O = a Sz wo 3 2s wm 5s S 3 2 wou O =) dp) Time (s) Fig. 3. Representative spectrograms from a microphone recording (airborne: top) and an accelerometer recording (substrate-borne: bottom) for a purring male that illustrates the broadband percussive sound present in many of our recordings. Percussive strikes (presum- ably with forelegs) appear as white vertical bars just before 1 second and at 2.5 seconds. The time is shown on the x-axis (seconds), and the colors (purple < red < orange < yellow < white) represent the power present at the various frequencies shown on the y-axis. 47 We also detected vibrations that were not associated with strid- ulation and were not associated with locomotion. These high-pow- er broadband vibrations were present in 19 of the 27 field-recorded individuals (Fig. 3) and appear percussive. Video footage suggested that the percussive bursts were produced through foreleg drum- ming: males could be seen striking the substrate with either the right or left foreleg repeatedly, and all males used both legs (Suppl. material 1: video). For example, the male in the Suppl. material 1: video makes several drums with his left leg, followed by one drum with his right leg and then a pause. For the 19 males that ex- hibited drumming, we randomly selected one drum from the first song that contained drumming, one drum from the second song that contained drumming, and one drum from the last song that contained drumming. Following the methods described above to extract data from the accelerometer track (substrate), we analyzed the peak frequency and amplitude of the three drums per male. We also counted the number of drums during each of the three songs. The drumming sounds had an average peak frequency of 105 + 19.5 kHz (mean + standard deviation) and an amplitude of 49 + 54 (Fig. 3). There was an average of 5.6 + 2.9 drums per song. Discussion This is the first documentation of substrate-borne vibration in T. oceanicus. We recorded vibrations in two different male morphs of T. oceanicus (typical and purring) that appear to be generated through the movement of the wings, as the pattern in the vibra- tional channel perfectly matches the airborne signal. When we compared typical and purring males, we found that typical males produced lower peak frequency sounds in both the airborne (Fig. 2a) and the vibrational channel (Fig. 2c). For amplitude, typical males produced sounds that were many times louder than purring males in the airborne channel (Fig. 2b), but there was no difference between morphs in amplitude in the vibrational chan- nel (Fig. 2d). As expected, purring males differed from typical males in the airborne channel, with a mean peak frequency of 4.7 kHz for typi- cal males and 6.0 kHz for purring males. These values are simi- lar to those of Tinghitella et al. (2018), who reported a median peak frequency of 5.0 kHz for typical males and 7.6 kHz for purr- ing males. Amplitude also matched previous results, with typical males producing significantly louder songs than purring males in the airborne channel. Because we used linear amplitude instead of decibels, which are on a logarithmic scale, we can linearly com- pare values of amplitude across male morphs. Thus, in the air- borne channel, the amplitude of the typical song was 38 times greater than that of the purring song (115 divided by 3). In the substrate-borne channel, purring males produced vibra- tions that were higher in frequency than typical males, but the dif- ference was not as dramatic as in the airborne channel; the effect size was 1.05 for substrate-borne compared to 1.83 in the airborne channel. As expected, the peak frequencies were much lower in the substrate-borne channel, ranging from 32 to 176 Hz. Because higher frequencies attenuate more quickly in soil, we would expect substrate-borne vibrations to be lower in frequency (Cokl and Vi- rant-Doberlet 2003, Caldwell 2014), and small-bodied species can produce low-frequency vibrations through stridulation (Cocroft and Rodriguez 2005). The frequency range measured for T. oceani- cus matches that of other known species of Orthoptera that send airborne and substrate-borne vibrations through wing stridula- tion: G. bimaculatus at 30-500 Hz (Weidemann and Keuper 1987) and T cantans at 0-800 Hz (Keuper and Kiihne 1983). We did not JOURNAL OF ORTHOPTERA RESEARCH 2021, 30(1) 48 detect a difference in amplitude between morphs in the substrate- borne channel. This suggests that the differences in wing morphol- ogy between morphs only affect amplitude in the airborne channel and not the substrate-borne channel. Purring males have a reduc- tion in the size of the harp and mirror (resonating structures) com- pared to typical males (Tinghitella et al. 2018 and unpublished). Thus, resonating structures on the wings (harp and mirror) play a key role in amplifying sound in the airborne channel but do not seem to affect the transfer of the vibration to the substrate. We found support for our hypothesis that wing movement produces both airborne signals and substrate-borne vibrations. Temporal components of song matched when we compared the microphone track to the accelerometer track, and these included start time, chirp length, trill length, and the interval between the chirp and the trill. These values were almost a perfect match in every category except for a few that differed by 0.001 millisec- onds, which can be attributed to human and equipment error. We were unable to distinguish between substrate-borne vibra- tions produced via coupling of the male to the substrate (through the legs or abdomen) or induction of airborne waves to the sub- strate. Future work could make this distinction by not allowing the stridulating male to come in contact with the substrate or by adapting methods from Keuper and Kihne (1983) where differ- ent substrates were tested. Regardless of the mechanism (direct coupling or induction of waves to the substrate), our results sup- port the claim that the production of an acoustic signal unavoid- ably excites vibrations in the substrate on which a signaler is rest- ing (Caldwell 2014), and our work adds to the growing literature documenting coupled airborne and substrate-borne sound pro- duced by stridulation in Orthopterans (e.g., T: cantans, Latimer and Schatral 1983; T. viridissima, Schatral et al. 1985; G. bimacula- tus, Weidemann and Keuper 1987). The big question remaining is whether female T: oceanicus can sense the substrate-borne vibrations and whether they affect mate choice. First, the ability to detect, receive, and translate vibrations is ancient and ubiquitous, found throughout vertebrates and ar- thropods (Hoy and Robert 1996, Cocroft and Rodriguez 2005, Hill 2009, Lakes-Harlan and Straub 2014). In T: oceanicus, males and females use cerci to detect sounds between 0 and 1000 Hz (Hoy et al. 1982, Pollack et al. 1998). Air flow and air currents produced by wing movements rather than stridulation can produce low-fre- quency (<70 Hz) air vibrations that are detectable through cerci up to a few centimeters away in Gryllus bimaculatus (Kamper and Dambach 1985). Similarly, it has been suggested that air currents produced by stridulating T: oceanicus males can be perceived by females, but substrate-borne vibration was not measured in that study (Pollack et al. 1998). The subgenual organ in the tibia is also a likely candidate for the detection of low-frequency vibrations in T. oceanicus (Lakes-Harlan and Straufs 2014), as it is used to detect substrate-borne vibrations as low as 50 Hz in related species (e.g,, Gryllus bimaculatus and Gryllus campestris; Dambach 1989), and the substrate-borne vibrations we measured are above that 50 Hz threshold (85.5 Hz for typical and 118.0 Hz for purring). When discussing the detectability of these substrate-borne vi- brations for T. oceanicus, we should consider amplitude and the average distance that vibrations travel through soil. While we did not measure female response to substrate-borne vibration in this study, Pollack et al. (1998) used electrodes inserted into abdomi- nal interneurons from the cerci to show that female T. oceanicus can “hear” low-frequency sounds produced by male wing move- ment from 2 cm away. We measured vibrations in a courtship con- text where females are a few centimeters from a signaling male, E.D. BRODER, A.W. WIKLE, J.H. GALLAGHER AND R.M. TINGHITELLA well within the range of perception for T: oceanicus, and our ampli- tude measurements were adjusted for background noise (e.g., 9.5 represents a signal that exceeded background noise, 0). In other Orthopterans that produce both airborne and substrate-borne vi- brations via stridulation, G. major vibrations travel up to a meter through the soil from a signalling male’s burrow (Hill and Shadley 1997), T. cantans vibrations travel about a meter through plant stems (Keuper and Kiithne 1983), and G. bimaculatus vibrations are detectable 20 cm away in dry soil (Weidemann and Keuper 1987). Additionally, while the amplitudes we measured (purring = 8.9 + 1.0, typical = 10.1 + 1.1) should be detectable over the short distances associated with courtship (1-2 cm), it is possible that the percussive sounds we detected may travel further since the am- plitude was much greater (49 + 54). Following methods described by Hill and Shadley (2001), we plan to measure the distance over which substrate-borne vibrations are detectable in T. oceanicus in a future study. While the first step is to explore the ability of T. oceanicus to de- tect the vibrations that courting males are producing, the next step is to assess the use of vibrations in mate choice. This could be ex- plored using playback experiments with an electrodynamic shaker played alone or in combination with an airborne signal (Cocroft and Rodriguez 2005). There are numerous examples of substrate- borne vibrational signals playing a key role in mate choice in Or- thoptera (e.g., de Luca and Morris 1998, Cocroft and Rodriguez 2005, Koéarek et al. 2010). In T: oceanicus, it has been suggested that the cerci detection system evolved to detect terrestrial preda- tors like frogs (Hoy et al. 1982). It is possible that this predator detection system has been co-opted for use in mate choice. Other studies have used phylogenetic approaches to demonstrate that vibrational communication evolved from existing responses like locomotion (Scott et al. 2010) and a startle response (ter Hofstede et al. 2015). We might expect the rapid evolution of acoustic song (silent morph, Zuk et al. 2006; purring morph, Tinghitella et al. 2018) and the evolution of relaxed female preferences (Tinghitella and Zuk 2009) in T. oceanicus to shape both the substrate-borne vibrations and the sensory capabilities of and preference for those vibrations. Additionally, selection may differ between the airborne and substrate-borne channels; louder airborne songs are preferred by T. oceanicus females, but there is no difference in amplitude between morphs in the substrate-borne channel. Thus, investiga- tions of whether substrate-borne vibrations play a role in T. oceani- cus courtship should compare populations with different morphs, simultaneously measure airborne song, and monitor both detec- tion and preference over time in this rapidly evolving system. While valuable, this work has limitations. First, we used filter paper as our substrate, which was appropriate for our question since we compared morphs measured on an identical substrate. However, because the composition of any substrate imposes fre- quency filters on substrate-borne vibrations, we expect the spectral characteristics found in this study to differ from those recorded on natural substrates. Future works should measure and characterize these substrate-borne vibrations on natural and variable substrates (e.g., following Hill 2001). Second, while our interindividual vari- ation was low, suggesting that our sample size adequately cap- tured the patterns between morphs, a larger sample across more populations would give a more holistic view of substrate-born vi- bration in Hawaiian T. oceanicus. More-advanced equipment, such as a high-speed video camera and a laser vibrometer, would also allow us to tease apart the way in which wing movement translates to substrate-borne vibrations (air movement or contact between file and scraper traveling through the legs). JOURNAL OF ORTHOPTERA RESEARCH 2021, 30(1) E.D. BRODER, A.W. WIKLE, J.H. GALLAGHER AND R.M. TINGHITELLA As with most discoveries, this work lays the groundwork for future questions. In addition to exploring how T. oceanicus females detect and use substrate-borne vibrations, we must also recognize that communication happens in a network (Virant-Doberlet et al. 2014, 2019). Our network has multiple senders (signaling males), intended receivers (female crickets), and unintended receivers that are conspecifics (satellite males) and heterospecifics (parasitoid flies). The eavesdropping predators, O. ochracea, are an important driver of the rapid evolution of airborne signals in T: oceanicus, and because they are airborne aerial predators, vibration may be hid- den from this natural selection. However, vibrations are likely sus- ceptible to a different set of unintended predatory receivers, such as spiders (Virant-Doberlet et al. 2019). Eavesdropping male crick- ets may also be able to use vibration to locate singing males for satellite behavior. Finally, future work should also explore other parts of substrate-borne vibrations, such as the broadband per- cussive taps produced by the forelegs. RMT has conducted court- ship trials in the field for the past 15 years and did not observe this behavior until 2017. To date, no literature has been published documenting this behavior in T: oceanicus, but drumming (percus- sive) behavior is a commonly utilized mechanism of vibrational signal production among diverse arthropods (e.g., fiddler crabs, Aicher and Tautz 1990; termites, Rohrig et al. 1999; wetas, Gw- ynne 2004; mole crickets, Hayashi et al. 2018; reviewed in Hill 2012), and drumming may be important even when there are oth- er mechanisms producing substrate-borne vibrations (in our case, wing stridulation), as seen in a stink bug that uses tremulation, buzzing, abdomen vibration, and drumming (Kavéié et al. 2013). Finally, drumming may function in both the substrate-borne and airborne channels (Hill 2012). This work is a first look at substrate-borne vibration in T. oce- anicus and answers calls by many to explore communication in the vibrational channel (e.g., Hill 2001, Cocroft and Rodriguez 2005). This work lays the foundation for future research on variation in substrate-borne vibration in T. oceanicus, intended and unintend- ed receiver responses to these vibrations, and the coevolution of substrate-borne vibrations with other courtship signals, such as airborne and chemical signals. Acknowledgements This work was funded in part by the Orthopterist Society through a Cohn Grant awarded to EDB. Additional support was provided by an NSF grant awarded to RMT (NSF IOS 1846520) and a Stoffel Fund for Excellence in Scientific Inquiry Grant awarded to EDB. While on Molokai, we were hosted and sup- ported by the Kalaupapa National Historical Park, Dr. Paul Hosten, and Pastor Richard Miller. We would like to thank the Kasey Fowler-Finn lab for lending us an accelerometer set-up for this project. We would also like to thank those who helped us in the field, including Claudia Hallagan, Jake Wilson, and Brooke Washburn. References Aicher B, Tautz J (1990) Vibrational Communication in the Fiddler Crab, Uca pugilator. |. Signal Transmission Through the Substratum. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 166: 345-353. https://doi.org/10.1007/ BF00204807 Balakrishnan R, Pollack G (1997) The role of antennal sensory cues in female responses to courting males in the cricket Teleogryllus oce- anicus. Journal of Experimental Biology 200: 511-522. https://doi. org/10.1111/1365-2435.13321 49 Belwood JJ, Morris GK (1987) Bat predation and its influence on calling behavior in neotropical katydids. Science 238: 64-67. https://doi. org/10.1126/science.238.4823.64 Benediktov AA (2009) Vibration communication in orthopteroid in- sects (Orthoptera) from suborder Caelifera. Moscow University Biological Sciences Bulletin 64: 126-128. https://doi.org/10.3103/ S$0096392509030080 Caldwell MS (2014) Interactions between airborne sound and substrate vibration in animal communication. In: Cocroft RB, Gogala M, Hill PSM, Wessel A (Eds) Studying Vibrational Communication. Springer, Berlin, 65-92. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-43607-3_6 Cocroft RB, Gogala M, Hill PSM, Wessel A (2014) Fostering Research Pro- gress in a Rapidly Growing Field. In: Cocroft RB, Gogala M, Hill PSM, Wessel A (Eds) Studying Vibrational Communication. Springer, Ber- lin, 12 pp. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-43607-3_1 Cocroft RB, Rodriguez RL (2005) The behavioral ecology of insect vi- brational communication. Bioscience 55: 323-334. https://doi. org/10.1641/0006-3568(2005)055[0323:TBEOIV]2.0.CO;2 Cohen J (1977) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Aca- demic Press, New York. Cokl A, Virant-Doberlet M (2003) Communication with substrate-borne signals in small plant-dwelling insects. Annual Review of Entomology 48: 29-50. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ento.48.091801.112605 Dambach M (1989) Vibrational responses. In Huber FE Moore TE, Loher W (Eds) Cricket Behaviour and Neurobiology. Cornell University Press, 179-197. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80655-4 de Luca PA, Morris G (1998) Courtship communication in meadow ka- tydids: female preference for large male vibrations. Behaviour 135: 777-794. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853998792640422 Dierkes S, Barth FG (1995) Mechanism of signal production in the vi- bratory communication of the wandering spider Cupiennius getazi (Arachnida, Araneae). Journal of Comparative Physiology A 176: 31-44. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00197750 Elias DO, Sivalinghem S, Mason AC, Andrade MC, Kasumovic MM (2010) Vi- bratory communication in the jumping spider Phidippus clarus: substrate- borne courtship signals are important for male mating success. Ethol- ogy 116: 990-998. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01815.x Gwynne DT (2004) Reproductive behavior of ground weta (Orthoptera: Anostostomatidae): drumming behavior, nuptial feeding, post-copu- latory guarding and maternal care. Journal of the Kansas Entomologi- cal Society 77: 414-428. https://doi.org/10.2317/E-34.1 Hayashi Y, Yoshimura J, Roff DA, Kumita T, Shimizu A (2018). Four types of vibration behaviors in a mole cricket. PLoS ONE 13: e0204628. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0204628 Hebets EA, Papaj DR (2005) Complex signal function: developing a frame- work of testable hypotheses. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 57: 197-214. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-004-0865-7 Higham JP, Hebets EA (2013) An introduction to multimodal communica- tion. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 67: 1381-1388. https:// doi.org/10.1007/s00265-013-1590-x Hill PSM (2001) Vibration and animal communication: a review. Ameri- can Zoologist 41: 1135-1142. https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/41.5.1135 Hill PSM (2009) How do animals use substrate-borne vibrations as an in- formation source? Naturwissenschaften 96: 1355-1371. https://doi. org/10.1007/s00114-009-0588-8 Hill PSM (2012) Do insect drummers actually drum? Studying vibrational communication across taxa. Mitteilungen der Deutschen Gesellschaft fiir Allgemeine und Angewandte Entomologie 18: 603-611. Hill PSM, Shadley JR (1997). Substrate vibration as a component of a call- ing song. Naturwissenschaften 84: 60-463. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s001140050429 Hill PS, Virant-Doberlet M, Wessel A (2019) What Is Biotremology? In: Hill PSM, Lakes-Harlan R, Mazzoni V, Narins PM, Virant-Doberlet M, Wessel A (Eds) Biotremology: Studying Vibrational Behavior. Spring- er, Cham, 15-25. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-22293-2_2 Hoy RR, Robert D (1996) Tympanal hearing in insects. Annual Review of Entomology 41: 433-450. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev. en.41.010196.002245 JOURNAL OF ORTHOPTERA RESEARCH 2021, 30(1) 50 Hoy RR, Pollack GS, Moiseff A (1982) Species-recognition in the field crick- et, Teleogryllus oceanicus: behavioral and neural mechanisms. Ameri- can Zoologist 22: 597-607. https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/22.3.597 JMP, Version 14 (1989-2019) JMP, Version 14. SAS Institute Inc., Cary. Kamper G, Dambach M (1985) Low-frequency airborne vibrations gener- ated by crickets during singing and aggression. Journal of Insect Phys- iology 31: 925-929. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1910(85)90026-5 Kavéié A, Cokl A, Laumann RA, Blassioli-Moraes MC, Borges M (2013) Tremulatory and abdomen vibration signals enable communication through air in the stink bug Euschistus heros. PLoS ONE 8: e56503. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0056503 Keuper A, Kithne R (1983) The acoustic behaviour of the bushcricket Tet- tigonia cantans. II. Transmission of airborne-sound and vibration signals in the biotope. Behavioral Processes 8: 125-145. https://doi. org/10.1016/0376-6357(83)90002-5 Koéarek P (2010) Substrate-borne vibrations as a component of intraspecif- ic communication in the groundhopper Tetrix ceperoi. Journal of Insect Behavior 23: 348-363. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10905-010-9 218-8 Lakes-Harlan R, Strauf$ J (2014) Functional morphology and evolutionary diversity of vibration receptors in insects. In: Cocroft RB, Gogala M, Hill PSM, Wessel A (Eds) Studying Vibrational Communication. Springer, Berlin, 277-302. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-43607-3_14 Latimer W, Schatral A (1983) The acoustic behaviour of the bushcricket Tettigonia cantans 1. Behavioural responses to sound and vibration. Behavioural Processes 8: 113-124. https://doi.org/10.1016/0376- 6357(83)90001-3 Masters WM (1979) Insect disturbance stridulation: its defensive role. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 5: 187-200. https://doi. org/10.1007/BF00293305 Morris GK, Mason AC, Wall P (1994) High ultrasonic and tremulation signals in neotropical katydids (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). Journal of Zoology 233: 129-163. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.1994. tb05266.x Partan SR (2017) Multimodal shifts in noise: switching channels to com- municate through rapid environmental change. Animal Behaviour 124: 325-337. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.08.003 Pascoal S, Mendrok M, Wilson AJ, Hunt J, Bailey NW (2017) Sexual se- lection and population divergence II. Divergence in different sexual traits and signal modalities in field crickets (Teleogryllus oceanicus). Evolution 71: 1614-1626. https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.13239 Pollack GS, Givois V, Balakrishnan R (1998) Air-movement signals are not required for female mounting during courtship in the cricket Teleog- ryllus oceanicus. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 183: 513-518. https://doi.org/10.1007/s003590050276 Rajaraman K, Godthi V, Pratap R, Balakrishnan R (2015) A novel acous- tic-vibratory multimodal duet. Journal of Experimental Biology 218: 3042-3050. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.122911 Rajaraman K, Nair A, Dey A, Balakrishnan R (2018) Response Mode Choice in a Multimodally Duetting Paleotropical Pseudophylline Bushcricket. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution 6: e172. https://doi. org/10.3389/fevo.2018.00172 Rohrig A, Kirchner WH, Leuthold RH (1999) Vibrational alarm commu- nication in the African fungus-growing termite genus Macrotermes (Isoptera, Termitidae). Insectes Sociaux 46: 71-77. https://doi. org/10.1007/s000400050115 Schatral A, Latimer W, Kalmring K (1985) Role of the song for spatial dis- persion and agonistic contacts in male bushcrickets. In: Kalmring K, Elsner N (Eds) Acoustic and vibrational communication in insects: proceedings from the XVII. International Congress of Entomology held at the University of Hamburg, Berlin. Schneider WT, Rutz C, Hedwig B, Bailey NW (2018) Vestigial singing be- haviour persists after the evolutionary loss of song in crickets. Biology letters 14: e20170654. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2017.0654 E.D. BRODER, A.W. WIKLE, J.H. GALLAGHER AND R.M. TINGHITELLA Scott JL, Kawahara AY, Skevington JH, Yen SH, Sami A, Smith ML, Yack JE (2010) The evolutionary origins of ritualized acoustic signals in caterpillars. Nature Communications 1: 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1038/ ncomms1002 Simmons LW, Tinghitella RM, Zuk M (2010) Quantitative genetic varia- tion in courtship song and its covariation with immune function and sperm quality in the field cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus. Behavioral Ecology 21: 1330-1336. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arq154 Stolting H, Moore TE, Lakes-Harlan R (2002) Substrate vibrations during acoustic signalling in the cicada Okanagana rimosa. Journal of Insect Science 2: 1-2. https://doi.org/10.1093/jis/2.1.2 Stritih N, Cokl A (2014) The role of frequency in vibrational communica- tion of Orthoptera. In: Cocroft RB, Gogala M, Hill PSM, Wessel A (Eds) Studying Vibrational Communication. Springer, Berlin, 375- 393. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-43607-3_19 ter Hofstede HM, Schoéneich S, Robillard T, Hedwig B (2015) Evolution of a communication system by sensory exploitation of startle be- havior. Current Biology 25: 3245-3252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. cub.2015.10.064 Tinghitella RM, Broder ED, Gurule-Small GA, Hallagan C, Wilson J (2018) Purring Crickets: The evolution of a novel sexual signal. The American Naturalist 192: 773-782. https://doi.org/10.1086/700116 Tinghitella RM, Broder ED, Gallagher JH, Wikle AW, Zonana DM (2021) Responses of intended and unintended receivers to a novel sexual sig- nal suggest clandestine communication. Nature Communications 12: 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-20971-5 Tinghitella RM, Zuk M (2009) Asymmetric mating preferences accommo- dated the rapid evolutionary loss of a sexual signal. Evolution: Inter- national Journal of Organic Evolution 63: 2087-2098. https://doi. org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.2009.00698.x Virant-Doberlet M, Kuhelj A, Polajnar J, Sturm R (2019) Predator-prey interactions and eavesdropping in vibrational communication net- works. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution 7: 1-15. https://doi. org/10.3389/fevo.2019.00203 Virant-Doberlet M, Mazzoni V, De Groot M, Polajnar J, Lucchi A, Symond- son WO, Cokl A (2014) Vibrational communication networks: eaves- dropping and biotic noise. In: Cocroft RB, Gogala M, Hill PSM, Wes- sel A (Eds) Studying Vibrational Communication. Springer, Berlin, 93-123. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-43607-3_7 Weidemann S, Keuper A (1987) Influence of vibratory signals on the pho- notaxis of the gryllid Gryllus bimaculatus DeGeer (Ensifera: Gryllidae). Oecologia 74: 316-318. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00379376 Zuk M, Kolluru GR (1998) Exploitation of sexual signals by predators and parasitoids. The Quarterly Review of Biology 73: 415-438. https:// doi.org/10.1086/420412 Zuk M, Rotenberry JI, Tinghitella RM (2006) Silent Night: Adaptive disappearance of a sexual signal in a parasitized population of field crickets. Biology Letters 2: 521-524. https://doi.org/10.1098/ rsbl.2006.0539 Supplementary material 1 Authors: E Dale Broder, Aaron W. Wikle, James H. Gallagher, Rob- in M. Tinghitella Data type: multimedia Copyright notice: This dataset is made available under the Open Database License _ (http://opendatacommons.org/licenses/ odbl/1.0/). The Open Database License (ODbL) is a license agreement intended to allow users to freely share, modify, and use this Dataset while maintaining this same freedom for others, provided that the original source and author(s) are credited. Link: https://doi.org/10.3897/jor.30.47778.suppl1 JOURNAL OF ORTHOPTERA RESEARCH 2021, 30(1)